Full Download Evidence-Based Physical Diagnosis 4th Edition Steven Mcgee PDF DOCX
Full Download Evidence-Based Physical Diagnosis 4th Edition Steven Mcgee PDF DOCX
com
https://textbookfull.com/product/evidence-based-
physical-diagnosis-4th-edition-steven-mcgee/
https://textbookfull.com/product/symptom-to-diagnosis-an-evidence-
based-guide-4th-edition-scott-d-c-stern/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/evidence-based-physical-examination-
handbook-1st-edition-kate-gawlik/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/evidence-based-diagnosis-an-
introduction-to-clinical-epidemiology-thomas-b-newman/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/introduction-to-electronic-defense-
systems-filippo-neri/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/quantum-theory-groups-and-
representations-an-introduction-1st-edition-peter-woit-auth/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/ethics-beyond-the-limits-new-essays-
on-bernard-williams-ethics-and-the-limits-of-philosophy-sophie-grace-
chappell/
textbookfull.com
Not In Your Genes The Real Reasons Children Are Like Their
Parents 1st Edition Oliver James
https://textbookfull.com/product/not-in-your-genes-the-real-reasons-
children-are-like-their-parents-1st-edition-oliver-james/
textbookfull.com
https://textbookfull.com/product/practical-guide-to-oracle-sql-t-sql-
and-mysql-1st-edition-preston-zhang/
textbookfull.com
Dorian Yates From the Shadow Official Biography Kaspa
Hazlewood
https://textbookfull.com/product/dorian-yates-from-the-shadow-
official-biography-kaspa-hazlewood/
textbookfull.com
4th Edition
Evidence-Based
Physical Diagnosis
Steven McGee, MD
Professor of Medicine,
University of Washington School of Medicine,
Seattle, Washington
1600 John F. Kennedy Blvd.
Ste 1800
Philadelphia, PA 19103-2899
No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, elec-
tronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage and retrieval
system, without permission in writing from the publisher. Details on how to seek permission, further
information about the Publisher’s permissions policies and our arrangements with organizations such
as the Copyright Clearance Center and the Copyright Licensing Agency, can be found at our web-
site: www.elsevier.com/permissions.
This book and the individual contributions contained in it are protected under copyright by the
Publisher (other than as may be noted herein).
Notices
Knowledge and best practice in this field are constantly changing. As new research and experi-
ence broaden our understanding, changes in research methods, professional practices, or medical
treatment may become necessary.
Practitioners and researchers must always rely on their own experience and knowledge in
evaluating and using any information, methods, compounds, or experiments described herein.
In using such information or methods they should be mindful of their own safety and the safety
of others, including parties for whom they have a professional responsibility.
With respect to any drug or pharmaceutical products identified, readers are advised to check
the most current information provided (i) on procedures featured or (ii) by the manufacturer of
each product to be administered, to verify the recommended dose or formula, the method and
duration of administration, and contraindications. It is the responsibility of practitioners, relying
on their own experience and knowledge of their patients, to make diagnoses, to determine dosages
and the best treatment for each individual patient, and to take all appropriate safety precautions.
To the fullest extent of the law, neither the Publisher nor the authors, contributors, or editors,
assume any liability for any injury and/or damage to persons or property as a matter of prod-
ucts liability, negligence or otherwise, or from any use or operation of any methods, products,
instructions, or ideas contained in the material herein.
Printed in
Steven McGee, MD
February 2016
vii
Introduction to the
First Edition
The purpose of this book is to explore the origins, pathophysiology, and diagnostic
accuracy of many of the physical signs currently used in adult patients. We have
a wonderfully rich tradition of physical diagnosis, and my hope is that this book
will help to square this tradition, now almost two centuries old, with the realities
of modern diagnosis, which often rely more on technologic tests, such as clinical
imaging and laboratory testing. The tension between physical diagnosis and tech-
nologic tests has never been greater. Having taught physical diagnosis for 20 years,
I frequently observe medical students purchasing textbooks of physical diagnosis
during their preclinical years, to study and master traditional physical signs, but
then neglecting or even discarding this knowledge during their clinical years, after
observing that modern diagnosis often takes place at a distance from the bedside.
One can hardly fault a student who, caring for a patient with pneumonia, does not
talk seriously about crackles and diminished breath sounds when all of his teachers
are focused on the subtleties of the patient’s chest radiograph. Disregard for physi-
cal diagnosis also pervades our residency programs, most of which have formal x-ray
rounds, pathology rounds, microbiology rounds, and clinical conferences addressing
the nuances of laboratory tests. Very few have formal physical diagnosis rounds.
Reconciling traditional physical diagnosis with contemporary diagnostic stan-
dards has been a continuous process throughout the history of physical diagnosis. In
the 1830s the inventor of topographic percussion, Professor Pierre Adolphe Piorry,
taught that there were nine distinct percussion sounds which he used to outline the
patient’s liver, heart, lungs, stomach, and even individual heart chambers or lung
cavities. Piorry’s methods flourished for more than a century and once filled 200-
page manuals,1 although nowadays, thanks to the introduction of clinical imaging
in the early 1900s, the only vestige of his methods is percussion of the liver span.
In his 1819 A Treatise on Diseases of the Chest,2 Laennec wrote that lung ausculta-
tion could detect “every possible case” of pneumonia. It was only a matter of 20
years before other careful physical diagnosticians tempered Laennec’s enthusiasm
and pointed out that the stethoscope had diagnostic limitations.3 And, for most
of the 20th century, expert clinicians believed that all late systolic murmurs were
benign, until Barlow et al in 1963 showed they often represented mitral regurgita-
tion, sometimes of significant severity.4
There are two contemporary polar opinions of physical diagnosis. Holding the
less common position are clinicians who believe that all traditional physical signs
remain accurate nowadays, and these clinicians continue to quiz students about
Krönig isthmus and splenic percussion signs. A more common position is that
physical diagnosis has little to offer the modern clinician and that traditional signs,
though interesting, cannot compete with the accuracy of our more technologic
diagnostic tools. Neither position, of course, is completely correct. I hope this book,
by examining the best evidence comparing physical signs to current diagnostic
standards, will bring clinicians to a more appropriate middle-ground: that physical
diagnosis is a reliable diagnostic tool that can still help clinicians with many, but
not all, clinical problems.
ix
x INTRODUCTION TO THE FIRST EDITION
Steven McGee, MD
July 2000
REFERENCES
1. Weil A. Handbuch und Atlas der Topographischen Perkussion. Leipzig: F. C. W. Vogel; 1880.
2. Laennec RTH. A Treatise on the Diseases of the Chest (Facsimile Edition by Classics of
Medicine Library). London: T. & G. Underwood; 1821.
3. Addison T. The difficulties and fallacies attending physical diagnosis of diseases of the
chest. In: Wilks S, Daldy TM, eds. A Collection of the Published Writings of the Late Thomas
Addison (Facsimile Edition by Classics of Medicine Library). London: The New Sydenham
Society; 1846:242.
4. Barlow JB, Pocock WA, Marchand P, Denny M. The significance of late systolic murmurs.
Am Heart J. 1963;66(4):443–452.
PART 1
INTRODUCTION
CHAPTER 1
What Is Evidence-Based
Physical Diagnosis?
When clinicians diagnose disease, their intent is to place the patient’s experience
into a particular category (or diagnosis), a process implying specific pathogenesis,
prognosis, and treatment. This procedure allows clinicians to explain what is hap-
pening to patients and to identify the best way to restore the patient’s health. A
century ago, such categorization of disease rested almost entirely on empiric obser-
vation—what clinicians saw, heard, and felt at the patient’s bedside. Although
some technologic testing was available then (e.g., microscopic examination of spu-
tum and urine), its role in diagnosis was meager, and almost all diagnoses were based
on traditional examination (Fig. 1.1). For example, if patients presented a century
ago with complaints of fever and cough, the diagnosis of lobar pneumonia rested
on the presence of the characteristic findings of pneumonia—fever, tachycardia,
tachypnea, grunting respirations, cyanosis, diminished excursion of the affected
side, dullness to percussion, increased tactile fremitus, diminished breath sounds
(and later bronchial breath sounds), abnormalities of vocal resonance (bronchoph-
ony, pectoriloquy, and egophony), and crackles. If these findings were absent, the
patient did not have pneumonia. Chest radiography played no role in diagnosis
because it was not widely available until the early 1900s.
Modern medicine, of course, relies on technology much more than medicine
did a century ago (to our patients’ advantage), and for many modern categories of
disease, the diagnostic standard is a technologic test (see Fig. 1.1). For example, if
patients present today with fever and cough, the diagnosis of pneumonia is based
on the presence of an infiltrate on the chest radiograph. Similarly, the diagnosis of
systolic murmurs depends on echocardiography and that of ascites on abdominal
ultrasonography. In these disorders, the clinician’s principal interest is the result
of the technologic test, and decisions about treatment depend much more on that
result than on whether the patient exhibits egophony, radiation of the murmur into
the neck, or shifting dullness. This reliance on technology creates tension for medi-
cal students, who spend hours mastering the traditional examination yet later learn
(when first appearing on hospital wards) that the traditional examination pales
in importance compared to technology, a realization prompting a fundamental
1
2 PART 1 INTRODUCTION
A CENTURY AGO:
Diagnostic standard:
Bedside observation
Technologic test
MODERN TIMES:
FIG. 1.1 EVOLUTION OF THE DIAGNOSTIC STANDARD. This figure compares the diag-
nostic process one century ago (top, before introduction of clinical imaging and modern laboratory
testing) to modern times (bottom), illustrating the relative contributions of bedside examination (gray
shade) and technologic tests (white shade) to the diagnostic standard. One century ago, most diag-
noses were defined by bedside observation, whereas today technologic standards have a much
greater diagnostic role. Nonetheless, there are many examples today of diagnoses based solely on
bedside findings (examples appear in the large gray shaded box). Evidence-based physical diagnosis,
on the other hand, principally addresses those diagnoses defined by technologic standards, because
it identifies those traditional findings that accurately predict the result of the technologic test, as
discussed throughout this book.
Visit https://textbookfull.com
now to explore a rich
collection of eBooks, textbook
and enjoy exciting offers!
CHAPTER 1 What Is Evidence-Based Physical Diagnosis? 3
question: What is the true diagnostic value of the traditional physical examination?
Is it outdated and best discarded? Is it completely accurate and underutilized? Is the
truth somewhere between these two extremes?
Examination of Fig. 1.1 indicates that diagnosis today is split into two parts. For
some categories of disease, the diagnostic standard still remains empiric observa-
tion—what the clinician sees, hears, and feels—just as it was for all diagnoses a
century ago. For example, how does a clinician know the patient has cellulitis? The
only way is to go to the patient’s bedside and observe fever and localized bright ery-
thema, warmth, swelling, and tenderness on the leg. There is no other way to make
this diagnosis (technologic or not). Similarly, there is no technologic standard for
Parkinson disease (during the patient’s life), Bell palsy, or pericarditis. All of these
diagnoses—and many others in the fields of dermatology, neurology, musculoskel-
etal medicine, and ophthalmology—are based entirely on empiric observation by
experienced clinicians; technology has a subordinate diagnostic role. In fact, the
principal reason medical students still must study and master the traditional exami-
nation is the dependence of many diagnoses on bedside findings.
The principal role of evidence-based physical examination, in contrast, is the
second category of diseases—that is, those whose categorization today is based on
technologic studies. Clinicians want to know the results of a chest radiograph when
diagnosing pneumonia, an echocardiogram when diagnosing systolic murmurs, and
an ultrasound when diagnosing ascites. For each of these problems, the evidence-
based approach compares traditional findings to the technologic standard and then
identifies those findings that increase or decrease the probability of disease (as
defined by the technologic standard), distinguishing them from unhelpful findings
that fail to change probability. Using this approach, the clinician will calculate the
Heckerling score* to predict the findings of the chest radiograph (Chapter 32),
define the topographic distribution of the murmur on the chest wall to predict the
findings of the echocardiogram (Chapter 43), and look for a fluid wave or edema to
predict the findings of the abdominal ultrasound examination (Chapter 51).
There are thus two distinct ways physical examination is applied at the bedside.
For many disorders—those still lacking a technologic standard—the clinician’s
observations define diagnosis. For other disorders—those based on technologic
tests—the clinician’s application of an evidence-based approach quickly identifies
the relatively few findings that predict the results of technologic standard. Both
approaches to bedside examination make physical examination more efficient,
accurate, and ultimately more relevant to the care of patients.
* The Heckerling score assigns one point to each of five independent predictors of pneumonia
that are present: temperature greater than 37.8° C, heart rate greater than 100 beats per min-
ute, crackles, diminished breath sounds, and absence of asthma (see Chapter 32).
PART 2
UNDERSTANDING
THE EVIDENCE
CHAPTER 2
Diagnostic Accuracy of
Physical Findings
I. INTRODUCTION
If a physical sign characteristic of a suspected diagnosis is present (i.e., positive
finding), that diagnosis becomes more likely; if the characteristic finding is absent
(i.e., negative finding), the suspected diagnosis becomes less likely. How much
these positive and negative results modify probability, however, is distinct for each
physical sign. Some findings, when positive, increase probability significantly, but
they change it little when negative. Other signs are more useful if they are absent,
because a negative finding practically excludes disease, although a positive one
changes probability very little.
5
6 PART 2 UNDERSTANDING THE EVIDENCE
Much of this book consists of tables that specifically describe how positive or
negative findings change the probability of disease, a property called diagnostic
accuracy. Understanding these tables first requires a review of four concepts: pre-
test probability, sensitivity, specificity, and LRs.
Holosystolic murmur:
Present 22 3 25
a b
c d
Absent 20 55 75
n1 n2
42 58
FIG. 2.1 2 × 2 TABLE. The total number of patients with disease (tricuspid regurgitation in this
example) is the sum of the first column, or n1 = a + c. The total number of patients without disease
is the sum of the second column, or n2 = b + d. The sensitivity of a physical finding (holosystolic
murmur at the left lower sternal edge, in this example) is the proportion of patients with disease
who have the finding [i.e., a/(a + c), or a/n1]. The specificity of a physical finding is the proportion of
patients without disease who lack the finding [i.e., d/(b + d), or d/n1]. The positive LR is the propor-
tion of patients with disease who have a positive finding (a/n1) divided by the proportion of patients
without disease who have a positive finding (b/n2), or sensitivity/(1 − specificity). The negative LR
is the proportion of patients with disease who lack the finding (c/n1) divided by the proportion of
patients without disease who lack the finding (d/n1), or (1 − sensitivity)/specificity. In this example,
the sensitivity is 0.52 (22/42), the specificity is 0.95 (55/58), the positive LR is 10.1 [(22/42)/(3/58)],
and the negative LR is 0.5 [(20/42)/(55/58)].
* The numbers used in this example are very close to those given in reference 23. See also
Chapter 46.
8 PART 2 UNDERSTANDING THE EVIDENCE
(i.e., tricuspid regurgitation, 42 patients) who have the characteristic murmur (i.e.,
the positive result, 22 patients), which is 22/42 = 0.52 or 52%. The specificity of
the holosystolic murmur is the proportion of patients without disease (i.e., no tri-
cuspid regurgitation, 58 patients) who lack the murmur (i.e., the negative result, 55
patients), which is 55/58 = 0.95 or 95%.
To recall how to calculate sensitivity and specificity, Sackett and others have
suggested helpful mnemonics: Sensitivity is represented as “PID” for “positivity in
disease” (an abbreviation normally associated with “pelvic inflammatory disease”),
and specificity is represented as “NIH” for “negativity in health” (an abbreviation
normally associated with the “National Institutes of Health”).24,25
A. DEFINITION
The LR of a physical sign is the proportion of patients with disease who have a par-
ticular finding divided by the proportion of patients without disease who also have
the same finding.
Probability of finding in patients with disease
LR =
Probability of the same finding in patients without disease
CHAPTER 2 Diagnostic Accuracy of Physical Findings 9
The adjectives positive or negative indicate whether that LR refers to the pres-
ence of the physical sign (i.e. positive result) or to the absence of the physical sign
(i.e., the negative result).
A positive LR, therefore, is the proportion of patients with disease who have a
physical sign divided by the proportion of patients without disease who also have
the same sign. The numerator of this equation—the proportion of patients with
disease who have the physical sign—is the sign’s sensitivity. The denominator—the
proportion of patients without disease who have the sign—is the complement of
specificity, or (1 − specificity). Therefore,
(sens)
Positive LR =
(1 − spec)
In our hypothetical study (see Fig. 2.1), the proportion of patients with tricuspid
regurgitation who have the murmur is 22/42, or 52.4% (i.e., the finding’s sensitiv-
ity), and the proportion of patients without tricuspid regurgitation who also have
the murmur is 3/58, or 5.2% (i.e., 1 − specificity). The ratio of these proportions
[i.e., (sensitivity)/(1 − specificity)] is 10.1, which is the positive LR for a holosys-
tolic murmur at the lower sternal border. This number indicates that patients with
tricuspid regurgitation are 10.1 times more likely to have the holosystolic murmur
than those without tricuspid regurgitation.
Similarly, the negative LR is the proportion of patients with disease lacking a
physical sign divided by the proportion of patients without disease also lacking the
sign. The numerator of this equation—the proportion of patients with disease
lacking the finding—is the complement of sensitivity, or (1 − sensitivity). The
denominator of the equation—the proportion of patients without disease lacking
the finding—is the specificity. Therefore,
(1 − sens)
Negative LR =
(spec)
Probability
Decrease Increase
No change
1. USING GRAPHS
A. PARTS OF THE GRAPH
Fig. 2.3 is an easy-to-use graph that illustrates the relationship between pre-test
probability (x-axis) and post-test probability (y-axis), given the finding’s LR. The
straight line bisecting the graph into an upper left half and a lower right half indi-
cates an LR of 1, which has no discriminatory value because, for findings with this
LR, post-test probability always equals pre-test probability. Physical findings that
argue for disease (i.e., LRs >1) appear in the upper left half of the graph; the larger
the value of the LR, the more the curve approaches the upper left corner. Physical
findings that argue against disease (i.e., LRs <1) appear in the lower right half of the
graph: the closer the LR is to zero, the more the curve approaches the lower right
corner.
In Fig. 2.3, the three depicted curves with LRs greater than 1 (i.e., LR = 2, 5, and
10) are mirror images of the three curves with LRs less than 1 (i.e., LR = 0.5, 0.2,
and 0.1). (This assumes the “mirror” is the line LR = 1.) This symmetry indicates
that findings with an LR of 10 argue as much for disease as those with an LR = 0.1
argue against disease (although this is true only for the intermediate pre-test prob-
abilities). Similarly, LR = 5 argues as much for disease as LR = 0.2 argues against it,
and LR = 2 mirrors LR = 0.5. Keeping these companion curves in mind will help
the clinician interpret the LRs throughout this book.†
† These companion pairs are easy to recall because they are inversely related: the inverse of 10
is 1/10 = 0.1; the inverse of 5 is 1/5 = 0.2; the inverse of 2 is 1/2 = 0.5.
CHAPTER 2 Diagnostic Accuracy of Physical Findings 11
0.9
0.8 10
0.7 5
Post-test probability
0.6 2
1
0.5
0.4 0.5
0.3 0.2
0.1
0.2
0.1
0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1
Pre-test probability
FIG. 2.3 PROBABILITY AND LIKELIHOOD RATIOS. The curves describe how pre-test
probability (x-axis) relates to post-test probability (y-axis), given the likelihood ratio (LR) for the phys-
ical finding. Only the curves for seven likelihood ratios are depicted (from LR = 0.1 to LR = 10).
If a finding has an LR other than one of these depicted seven curves, its position
can be estimated with little loss in accuracy. For example, the curve for LR = 4 lies
between LR = 5 and LR = 2, though it is closer to LR= 5 than it is to LR = 2.
1
0.9
0.8 10
5
Post-test probability 0.7
0.6 2
1
0.5
0.4 0.5
0.3 0.2
0.2 0.1
0.1
0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1
Pre-test probability
1
0.9
0.8 10
5
Post-test probability
0.7
2
0.6
1
0.5
0.4 0.5
0.3 0.2
0.2 0.1
0.1
0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1
Pre-test probability
FIG. 2.4 PROBABILITY AND LIKELIHOOD RATIOS: PATIENTS WITH PULMONARY
HYPERTENSION. In our hypothetical clinician’s practice, 42% of patients with pulmonary hyper-
tension have the complication of tricuspid regurgitation (i.e., pre-test probability is 42%). To use the
curves, the clinician finds 0.42 on the x-axis and extends a line upward. The post-test probability
of tricuspid regurgitation is read off the y-axis where the vertical line intersects the curve of the
appropriate LR. The probability of tricuspid regurgitation if a holosystolic murmur is present at the
left lower sternal edge (LR = 10.1) is 88%; the probability if the finding is absent (LR = 0.5) is 27%.
These curves illustrate an additional important point: physical signs are diag-
nostically most useful when they are applied to patients who have pre-test prob-
abilities in the intermediate range (i.e., 20% to 80%), because in this range, the
different LR curves diverge the most from the LR = 1 curve (thus significantly
increasing or decreasing probability). If instead the pre-test probability is already
CHAPTER 2 Diagnostic Accuracy of Physical Findings 13
Probability
Decrease Increase
–45% –30% –15% +15% +30% +45%
LRs 0.1 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10 LRs
very low or very high, all the LR curves cluster close to the line LR = 1 curve
in either the bottom left or upper right corners, thus with only a relatively small
impact on probability.
2. APPROXIMATING PROBABILITY
The clinician can avoid using graphs and instead approximate post-test probabil-
ity by remembering the following two points: First, the companion LR curves in
Fig. 2.3 are LR = 2 and LR = 0.5, LR = 5 and LR = 0.2, and LR = 10 and LR =
0.1. Second, the first three multiples of 15 are 15, 30, and 45. Using this rule, the
LRs of 2, 5, and 10 increase probability about 15%, 30%, and 45%, respectively
(Fig. 2.5). The LRs of 0.5, 0.2, and 0.1 decrease probability by about 15%, 30%,
and 45%, respectively.28 These estimates are accurate to within 5% to 10% of the
actual value, as long as the clinician rounds estimates over 100 to an even 100%
and estimates below zero to an even 0%.
Therefore, in our hypothetical patient with pulmonary hypertension, the find-
ing of a holosystolic murmur (LR = 10) increases the probability of tricuspid regur-
gitation from 42% to 87% (i.e., 42% + 45% = 87%, which is only 1% lower than
the actual value). The absence of the murmur (LR = 0.5) decreases the probability
of tricuspid regurgitation from 42% to 27% (i.e., 42% − 15% = 27%, which is iden-
tical to actual value).
Table 2.2 summarizes similar bedside estimates for all LRs between 0.1 and 10.0.
3. CALCULATING PROBABILITY
Post-test probability can also be calculated by first converting pre-test probability
(Ppre) into pre-test odds (Opre):
Ppre
Opre =
(1 − Ppre )
The pre-test odds (Opre) is multiplied by the LR of the physical sign to deter-
mine the post-test odds (Opost):
Opost = Opre × LR
Visit https://textbookfull.com
now to explore a rich
collection of eBooks, textbook
and enjoy exciting offers!
14 PART 2 UNDERSTANDING THE EVIDENCE
The post-test odds (Opost) converts back to post-test probability (Ppost), using
Opost
Ppost =
(1 + Opost )
2. ACCURACY
Using LRs to describe diagnostic accuracy is superior to describing it in terms of
sensitivity and specificity, because the previously explained mnemonics, SpPin
and SnNout, are sometimes misleading. For example, according to the mnemonic
SpPin, a finding with a specificity of 95% should argue conclusively for disease, but
CHAPTER 2 Diagnostic Accuracy of Physical Findings 15
it does so only if the positive LR for the finding is a high number. If the finding’s
sensitivity is 60%, the positive LR is 12 and the finding argues convincingly for
disease (i.e., consistent with the SpPin mnemonic); if the finding’s sensitivity is
only 10%, however, the positive LR is 2 and the post-test probability changes only
slightly (i.e., inconsistent with SpPin mnemonic). Similarly, a highly sensitive find-
ing argues convincingly against disease when absent (i.e., SnNout) only when its
calculated negative LR is close to zero.
3. LEVELS OF FINDINGS
Another advantage of LRs is that a physical sign measured on an ordinal scale (e.g.,
0, 1+, 2+, 3+) or continuous scale (e.g., blood pressure) can be categorized into dif-
ferent levels to determine the LR for each level, thereby increasing the accuracy of
the finding. Other examples include continuous findings such as heart rate, respira-
tory rate, temperature, and percussed span of the liver, and ordinal findings such as
intensity of murmurs and degree of edema.
For example, in patients with chronic obstructive lung disease (i.e., emphysema,
chronic bronchitis), breath sounds are typically faint. If the clinician grades the
intensity of breath sounds on a scale from 0 (absent) to 24 (very loud), based on
the methods discussed in Chapter 30,29,30 he or she can classify the patient’s breath
sounds into one of four groups: scores of 9 or less (very faint), 10 to 12, 13 to 15, or
greater than 15 (loud). Each category then has its own LR (Table 2.3): scores of 9 or
less significantly increase the probability of obstructive disease (LR = 10.2), whereas
scores greater than 15 significantly decrease it (LR = 0.1). Scores from 10 to 12 argue
somewhat for disease (LR = 3.6), and scores from 13 to 15 provide no diagnostic
information (LR not significantly different from 1). If the clinician instead identifies
breath sounds as simply “faint” or “normal/increased” (i.e., the traditional positive or
negative finding), the finding may still discriminate between patients with and with-
out obstructive disease, but it misses the point that the discriminatory power of the
sign resides mostly with scores less than 10 and greater than 15.
When findings are categorized into levels, the term specificity becomes meaning-
less. For example, the specificity of a breath sound score of 13 to 15 is 80%, which
means that 80% of patients without chronic airflow limitation have values other
than 13 to 15, though the “80%” does not convey whether most of these other
values are greater than 15 or less than 13. Similarly, when findings are put in more
than two categories, the LR descriptor negative is no longer necessary, because all
LRs are positive for their respective category.
4. COMBINING FINDINGS
A final advantage of LRs is that clinicians can use them to combine findings, which
is particularly important for those physical signs with positive LRs around 2 or nega-
tive LRs around 0.5, signs that by themselves have little effect on probability but
16 PART 2 UNDERSTANDING THE EVIDENCE
when combined have significant effects on probability. Individual LRs can be com-
bined—however, only if the findings are “independent.”
A. INDEPENDENCE OF FINDINGS
Independence means that the LR for the second finding does not change once the
clinician determines whether the first finding is present or absent. For some select
diagnostic problems, investigators have identified which findings are independent
of each other. These findings appear as components of “diagnostic scoring schemes”
in the tables throughout this book (e.g., Wells score for deep venous thrombosis).
For most physical findings, however, very little information is available about inde-
pendence, and the clinician must judge whether combining findings is appropriate.
One important indication is that most independent findings have unique patho-
physiology. For example, when considering pneumonia in patients with cough and
fever, the clinician could combine the findings of abnormal mental status and
diminished breath sounds, using the individual LR of each finding because abnor-
mal mental status and diminished breath sounds probably have separate patho-
physiology. Similarly, when considering heart failure in patients with dyspnea, the
clinician could combine the findings of elevated neck veins and the third heart
sound because these findings also have different pathophysiology.
Examples of findings whose individual LRs should not be combined (because the
findings share the same pathophysiology) are flank dullness and shifting dullness in
the diagnosis of ascites (both depend on intra-abdominal contents dampening the
vibrations of the abdominal wall during percussion), neck stiffness and the Kernig
sign in the diagnosis of meningitis (both are caused by meningeal irritation), and
edema and elevated neck veins in the diagnosis of heart failure (both depend on
elevated right atrial pressure).
Until more information is available, the safest policy for the clinician to follow
when combining LRs of individual findings is to combine no more than three find-
ings, all of which have distinct pathophysiology.
Please look for the icon throughout the print book, which indicates where the
online evidence-based calculator can be used.
REFERENCES
1. Eskelinen M, Ikonen J, Lipponen P. Contributions of history-taking, physical examina-
tion, and computer assistance to diagnosis of acute small-bowel obstruction: a prospective
study of 1333 patients with acute abdominal pain. Scand J Gastroenterol. 1994;29:715–721.
2. Brewer RJ, Golden GT, Hitch DC, Rudolf LE, Wangensteen SL. Abdominal pain: an
analysis of 1000 consecutive cases in a university hospital emergency room. Am J Surg.
1976;131:219–223.
3. Böhner H, Yang Z, Franke C, Verreet PR, Ohmann C. Simple data from history and
physical examination help to exclude bowel obstruction and to avoid radiographic studies
in patients with acute abdominal pain. Eur J Surg. 1998;164:777–784.
4. Stiell IG, Greenberg GH, McKnight RD, Nair RC, McDowell I, Worthington JR. A
study to develop clinical decision rules for the use of radiography in acute ankle injuries.
Ann Emerg Med. 1992;21(4):384–390.
5. Stiell IG, Greenberg GH, McKnight RD, et al. Decision rules for the use of radiogra-
phy in acute ankle injuries: refinement and prospective validation. J Am Med Assoc.
1993;269:1127–1132.
6. Heckerling PS, Tape TG, Wigton RS, et al. Clinical prediction rule for pulmonary infil-
trates. Ann Intern Med. 1990;113:664–670.
7. Wells PS, Anderson DR, Bormanis J, et al. Value of assessment of pretest probability of
deep-vein thrombosis in clinical management. Lancet. 1997;350:1795–1798.
8. Miron MJ, Perrier A, Bounameaux H. Clinical assessment of suspected deep vein
thrombosis: comparison between a score and empirical assessment. J Intern Med.
2000;247:249–254.
9. Anderson DR, Wells PS, MacLeod B, et al. Thrombosis in the emergency department.
Arch Intern Med. 1999;159:477–482.
10. Aschwanden M, Labs KH, Jeanneret C, Gehrig A, Jaeger KA. The value of rapid D-dimer
testing combined with structured clinical evaluation for the diagnosis of deep vein throm-
bosis. J Vasc Surg. 1999;30:929–935.
11. Funfsinn N, Caliezi C, Baiasiutti FD, et al. Rapid D-dimer testing and pre-test clinical
probability in the exclusion of deep venous thrombosis in symptomatic outpatients. Blood
Coagul Fibrinolysis. 2001;12(3):165–170.
12. Kearon C, Ginsberg JS, Douketis J, et al. Management of suspected deep venous throm-
bosis in outpatients by using clinical assessment and D-dimer testing. Ann Intern Med.
2001;135:108–111.
13. Oudega R, Hoes AW, Moons KGM. The Wells rule does not adequately rule out deep
venous thrombosis in primary care patients. Ann Intern Med. 2005;143:100–107.
14. Schutgens REG, Ackermark P, Haas FJLM, et al. Combination of a normal D-dimer con-
centration and a non-high pretest clinical probability score is a safe strategy to exclude
deep venous thrombosis. Circulation. 2003;107:593–597.
15. Tick LW, Ton E, van Voorthuizen T, et al. Practical diagnostic management of patients
with clinically suspected deep vein thrombosis by clinical probability test, compression
ultrasonography, and D-dimer test. Am J Med. 2002;113:630–635.
16. Chagnon I, Bounameaux H, Aujesky D, et al. Comparison of two clinical prediction rules
and implicit assessment among patients with suspected pulmonary embolism. Am J Med.
2002;113:269–275.
17. Miniati M, Bottai M, Monti S. Comparison of 3 clinical models for predicting the prob-
ability of pulmonary embolism. Medicine. 2005;84:107–114.
18. Wells PS, Anderson DR, Rodger M, et al. Excluding pulmonary embolism at the bedside
without diagnostic imaging: management of patients with suspected pulmonary embolism
presenting to the emergency department by using a simple clinical model and D-dimer.
Ann Intern Med. 2001;135:98–107.
19. Wolf SJ, McCubbin TR, Feldhaus KM, Faragher JP, Adcock DM. Prospective validation
of Wells criteria in the evaluation of patients with suspected pulmonary embolism. Ann
Emerg Med. 2004;44:503–510.
20. Newman LG, Waller J, Palestro J, et al. Unsuspected osteomyelitis in diabetic foot ulcers:
diagnosis and monitoring by leukocyte scanning with indium In 111 oxyquinoline. J Am
Med Assoc. 1991;266:1246–1251.
16.e1
16.e2 REFERENCES
21. Fleischer AE, Didyk AA, Woods JB, Burns SE, Wrobel JS, Armstrong DG. Combined
clinical and laboratory testing improves diagnostic accuracy for osteomyelitis in the dia-
betic foot. J Foot Ankle Surg. 2009;48(1):39–46.
22. Ertugrul BM, Savk O, Ozturk B, Cobanoglu M, Oncu S, Sakarya S. The diagnosis of
diabetic foot osteomyelitis: examination findings and laboratory values. Med Sci Monit.
2009;15(6):CR307–CR312.
23. Rahko PS. Prevalence of regurgitant murmurs in patients with valvular regurgitation
detected by Doppler echocardiography. Ann Intern Med. 1989;111:466–472.
24. Sackett DL, Richardson WS, Rosenberg W, Haynes RB. Evidence-Based Medicine: How to
Practice and Teach EBM. New York, NY: Churchill Livingstone; 1997.
25. Sackett DL, Haynes RB, Tugwell P. Clinical Epidemiology: A Basic Science for Clinical
Medicine. 1st ed. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Co; 1985.
26. Fagan TJ. Nomogram for Bayes’ theorem. N Engl J Med. 1975;293:257.
27. Grimes DA, Schulz KF. Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios. Lancet.
2005;365:1500–1505.
28. McGee S. Simplifying likelihood ratios. J Gen Intern Med. 2002;17:646–649.
29. Bohadana AB, Peslin R, Uffholtz H. Breath sounds in the clinical assessment of airflow
obstruction. Thorax. 1978;33:345–351.
30. Pardee NE, Martin CJ, Morgan EH. A test of the practical value of estimating breath
sound intensity: breath sounds related to measured ventilatory function. Chest.
1976;70(3):341–344.
Random documents with unrelated
content Scribd suggests to you:
Però, poco dopo, le preoccupazioni di Ruth parvero avverarsi: Martin
e il professor Caldwell s’erano appartati in un cantuccio: e, sebbene
Martin avesse perduto la fastidiosa manìa di far grandi gesti, l’occhio
critico di Ruth rilevò e biasimò l’ardore eccessivo della parola di lui,
la fiamma troppo viva dei suoi occhi, il rossore del viso acceso. Egli
mancava di decoro e di sangue freddo e contrastava singolarmente
col giovane professore d’inglese, suo compagno.
Ma Martin non si preoccupava punto delle apparenze; e non aveva
impiegato molto tempo a rilevare la cultura mentale dell’altro e ad
apprezzarne il corredo scientifico. Inoltre, il professore Caldwell era
diverso dal solito tipo di professore inglese. Martin voleva indurlo a
parlare di cose professionali, e sebbene dapprima trovasse delle
difficoltà, vi riuscì. Martin non capiva perchè la gente non volesse
parlare di cose della propria professione. — È assurda e ridicola, —
aveva dichiarato a Ruth la settimana precedente, — questa
ripugnanza a parlare di cose «del mestiere»: perchè uomini e donne
si riuniscono, se non per scambiare quanto di meglio hanno in essi?
E ciò che hanno di meglio, è tutto quanto li interessa: la loro
specialità, la loro ragione di vivere, ciò che li fa riflettere e sognare.
Immaginate il signor Butler che annuncia delle idee su Verlaine o
sull’arte drammatica tedesca, o sui romanzi di D’Annunzio?...
Sarebbe da morirne dalla noia! Da parte mia, se sono assolutamente
costretto ad ascoltare Butler, preferisco sentirlo parlar di codici, cioè
di cose ch’egli conosce meglio delle altre; e la vita è così breve, che
voglio ottenere da ogni creatura il massimo che può darmi.
— Ma, — aveva obbiettato Ruth. — esistono argomenti d’interesse
generale.
— E questo è il vostro errore, — aveva aggiunto lui. — In generale,
le persone hanno la tendenza a scimmiottar coloro di cui conoscono
la superiorità e ch’essi scelgono come modelli. E chi sono questi
modelli? Gli oziosi, i ricchi oziosi, i quali non sanno nulla,
generalmente, di ciò che sanno coloro che lavorano e s’annoieranno
mortalmente udendoli chiacchierare dei fatti loro. Così vien decretata
la convenzione secondo la quale tale genere di conversazione è un
parlare «professionale» anzi bottegaio, e che parlare di cose
professionali o bottegaie è tutt’altro che simpatico. Così gli oziosi
decidono anche nello stabilire quali sono le cose di genere non
bottegaio, delle quali si può parlare: l’ultima novità teatrale, il libro
d’attualità, il gioco, il bigliardo, i cocktails, l’automobile, le riunioni
ippiche, la pesca della trota, le partite di caccia grossa, lo yachting,
ecc., giacchè, notate bene, questi sono argomenti che gli oziosi
conoscono. Insomma, essi soltanto possono parlare di cose della
loro «bottega»; e il buffo è che molte persone intelligenti, e tutti
coloro che fanno finta di esserlo, permettono agli oziosi di imporre la
legge. Quanto a me, io desidero da un uomo quanto v’è di meglio in
lui, ciò che voi chiamate cose professionali, bottegaie, di mestiere, o
come vi pare.
E Ruth non aveva capito: questo assalto contro la Cosa Stabilita le
era parso molto arbitrario.
Dunque, Martin, comunicando al professor Caldwell un po’ della
propria intensità, l’aveva costretto a esprimere le sue idee. Passando
vicino a loro due, Ruth udì Martin che diceva:
— Certamente lei non professerà delle eresìe simili nell’Università di
California.
Il professore Caldwell alzò le spalle.
— È la parola dell’onesto contribuente e del politicante, capite!
Sacramento assegna gl’impieghi, e perciò noi facciamo dei
salamelecchi a Sacramento, dove il consiglio d’amministrazione dei
Reggenti possiede la stampa di tutt’e due i partiti.
— È chiaro; ma lei? — insistè Martin. — Lei dev’essere come un
pesce fuor d’acqua.
— Ce n’è pochi come me, nel pantano universitario. Evidentemente,
mi capita talvolta di sentirmi spaesato; sento che starei meglio a
Parigi, o in Grub Street, o in una grotta d’eremiti, o tra la più
scapigliata bohème, in qualche trattoria a buon mercato del
Quartiere latino, a predicare idee radicali davanti a un uditorio
tumultuoso. Veramente, io sono quasi sicuro d’esser nato radicale,
ma ecco!... ci sono troppe questioni di cui non sono certo. Divento
timido quando mi trovo di fronte la mia mingherlina personalità che
mi impedisce d’afferrare tutti gli elementi d’un problema, dei grandi
problemi umani, vitali.
E mentre egli seguitava a parlare, Martin s’accorse che l’altro
sussurrava la «Canzone dei Venti Alisei»: