100% found this document useful (4 votes)
34 views

Heartbeat Sensor Projects with PulseSensor: Prototyping Devices with Biofeedback 1st Edition Yury Gitman 2024 scribd download

Gitman

Uploaded by

maagwexiawei
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
100% found this document useful (4 votes)
34 views

Heartbeat Sensor Projects with PulseSensor: Prototyping Devices with Biofeedback 1st Edition Yury Gitman 2024 scribd download

Gitman

Uploaded by

maagwexiawei
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 66

Download Full Version ebookmass - Visit ebookmass.

com

Heartbeat Sensor Projects with PulseSensor:


Prototyping Devices with Biofeedback 1st Edition
Yury Gitman

https://ebookmass.com/product/heartbeat-sensor-projects-
with-pulsesensor-prototyping-devices-with-biofeedback-1st-
edition-yury-gitman/

OR CLICK HERE

DOWLOAD NOW

Discover More Ebook - Explore Now at ebookmass.com


Instant digital products (PDF, ePub, MOBI) ready for you
Download now and discover formats that fit your needs...

Creative Prototyping with Generative AI: Augmenting


Creative Workflows with Generative AI (Design Thinking)
1st Edition Patrick Parra Pennefather
https://ebookmass.com/product/creative-prototyping-with-generative-ai-
augmenting-creative-workflows-with-generative-ai-design-thinking-1st-
edition-patrick-parra-pennefather/
ebookmass.com

Arduino and Raspberry Pi Sensor Projects for the Evil


Genius 1st Edition Chin

https://ebookmass.com/product/arduino-and-raspberry-pi-sensor-
projects-for-the-evil-genius-1st-edition-chin/

ebookmass.com

micro:bit Projects with Python and Single Board Computers:


Building STEAM Projects with Code Club and Kids' Maker
Groups Martin Tan
https://ebookmass.com/product/microbit-projects-with-python-and-
single-board-computers-building-steam-projects-with-code-club-and-
kids-maker-groups-martin-tan/
ebookmass.com

The Use of Force in International Law: A Case-Based


Approach Tom Ruys (Editor)

https://ebookmass.com/product/the-use-of-force-in-international-law-a-
case-based-approach-tom-ruys-editor/

ebookmass.com
Discover Digital Libraries. Theory and Practice 1st
Edition Iris Xie

https://ebookmass.com/product/discover-digital-libraries-theory-and-
practice-1st-edition-iris-xie/

ebookmass.com

Elementary Linear Algebra 8th Edition Ron Larson

https://ebookmass.com/product/elementary-linear-algebra-8th-edition-
ron-larson/

ebookmass.com

Changing Tides (Sandcastle Cove Book 1) Amber Kelly

https://ebookmass.com/product/changing-tides-sandcastle-cove-
book-1-amber-kelly/

ebookmass.com

Concepts of Materials Science Sutton Frs

https://ebookmass.com/product/concepts-of-materials-science-sutton-
frs/

ebookmass.com

Fundamentals of Skeletal Radiology 5th Edition Clyde A.


Helm

https://ebookmass.com/product/fundamentals-of-skeletal-radiology-5th-
edition-clyde-a-helm/

ebookmass.com
Wildflowers for Anna Lee Kasey Kennedy

https://ebookmass.com/product/wildflowers-for-anna-lee-kasey-kennedy/

ebookmass.com
Random documents with unrelated
content Scribd suggests to you:
Although Palæolithic man worked so much in stone, he did not
build in it. Hence our knowledge of the kinds of shelters he made for
himself is almost nil. There are Upper Palæolithic “tectiform”
paintings which look as if they might be attempts to depict houses. It
is clear, moreover, that in this period the general development of the
mechanical arts was sufficiently advanced to allow of the
construction of some sort of rude edifices.
It is conceivable that as far back as the Lower Palæolithic simple
shelters of branches were constructed, or that skins may have been
hung over a few poles to keep off wind and rain. On account of the
perishable nature of the materials involved, it happens that there is
no proof either for or against such a supposition. It is possible that in
time, when patient excavations shall have revealed some particularly
well preserved site, the holes may yet be found in which the posts of
a Palæolithic hut were once set. In case of a fire, the carbonized
stumps might prove to have been preserved in place; or the butts of
the posts might have gradually rotted away and the space once
occupied by them have become filled with an earthy material of
different color and consistency from the surrounding soil. In this
lucky event, even the size and shape of the house might be
reconstructed from the relative positions of the post holes. From
evidence of just this sort some interesting ideas have actually been
obtained as to the houses and village plan of Neolithic European
peoples. Of course, the chances are much less that remains of this
sort would be preserved from the Palæolithic. But the method would
be equally applicable if favorable conditions offered; and it is in some
such way that we may hope in the future to learn a little about the
earliest habitations that mankind constructed. In any event the
example serves to illustrate the indirect and delicate means of which
the student of prehistory must consistently avail himself in his
reconstructions of the past; and gives reason to believe that all that
has been learned about early man in the last fifty years is very little
in comparison with what the ensuing generation and century will
bring to light.

79. Religion
It has already been said that knowledge of religion, a non-material
thing, can be preserved from the remote past only by the most
roundabout means. It is conceivable that the people of the Upper
Palæolithic spent at least as much time in ceremonial observances
as in working flint. Analogy with modern uncivilized tribes would
make us think that this is quite likely. But the stone tools have
remained lying in the earth, while the religious customs went out of
use thousands of years ago and the beliefs were forgotten. Yet this
is known: As far back as the Mousterian, thirty thousand years ago,
certain practices were being observed by the Neandertal race of
western Europe which modern savages observe in obedience to the
dictates of their religion. When these people of the Mousterian laid
away their dead, they put some of their belongings with them. When
existing nations do this, it is invariably in connection with a belief in
the continued existence of the soul after death. We may reasonably
conclude therefore that even in this long distant period human
beings had arrived at a crude recognition of the difference between
flesh and spirit; in short, religion had come into being. Even to say
that Neandertal man did not know whether his dead were dead,
implies his recognition of something different from life in the body, for
he recognized of course that the body had become different.
Whether the Neandertal race already held to the existence of spirits
distinct from man or superior to him, it is impossible to say.
The Upper Palæolithic Cro-Magnon peoples laid out the bodies of
their dead and sometimes folded them. They also sometimes
painted the bodies, and buried flint implements and food in the
graves. That is, funerary practices were becoming established. We
may assume that hand in hand with this development of
observances there went a growth of ritual and belief.

80. Palæolithic Art


The highest achievement of the men of the Old Stone Age is their
art. The perfection to which they carried this art is simply astounding
in view of the comparative meagerness of their civilization otherwise.
It is also remarkable how full-fledged this achievement sprang into
existence. The Lower Palæolithic seems to have been without a
trace of art. With the Aurignacian, simple carving and painting
appear; and while the acme of accomplishment was not reached
until the Magdalenian, the essential foundations of a graphic art of
high order were laid in the late Aurignacian.
The Upper Palæolithic people carved in ivory, bone, and horn;
they incised or engraved on flattened and rounded surfaces of the
same material; and they carved and painted the walls of caves. They
modeled at times in clay and perhaps in other soft materials, and
may have drawn or painted pictures on skins and on exposed rock
surfaces, for all we know; we can judge only by the remains that
have actually come down to us. This art is not a child-like, struggling
attempt to represent objects in the rough, nor is it a mere decorative
playing with geometric figures. These first human artists set boldly to
work to depict; and while their technique was simple, it was carried
to a remarkably high degree of perfection. A few bold strokes gave
the outlines of an animal, but they gave it with such fidelity that the
species can often be recognized at a glance. The Cro-Magnon
people must have developed a high power of mental concentration
to be able to observe and reproduce so closely. The most gifted
individuals perhaps practised assiduously to attain their facility.
Palæolithic art is very different from that of most modern savages.
The latter often work out decorative patterns of some complexity,
richness, and æsthetic value, but when they attempt to depict nature,
they usually fail conspicuously. The lines are crude and wavering.
Any head, body, and tail with four legs stands for almost any animal.
It is a reasonable representation of an abstraction that they
accomplish, not the delineation of what is characteristic in the visible
form. Both observer and painter, among most living savages, are
supposed to know beforehand that the drawing represents a fox and
not a bear. At most, some symbols are added, such as a bushy tail
for a fox or a fin for a whale. It is only in rare cases that any but
advanced nations break away from these primitive tendencies and
learn to draw things as they really appear. The ancient Egyptians
developed such a faculty, and among savages the Bushmen are
remarkably gifted, but, on the whole, successful realistic art is an
accomplishment of high civilization. It is therefore something of a
mystery how the Cro-Magnon men of the Aurignacian brought
themselves to do so well.

Fig. 20. Limestone statuette from


Willendorf, Austria. Characteristic
of Aurignacian treatment of the
female figure: the face and limbs
are abbreviated or only indicated;
the parts concerned with
reproduction are exaggerated.

In sculpture their first efforts were directed upon figurines. These


mostly represent the human female. The head, hands, and feet are
either absent or much abbreviated. In the body, those parts having to
do with reproduction and fecundity are usually heavily exaggerated,
but at the same time given with considerable skill (Fig. 20). It is likely
that these statuettes served some religious cult. At any rate, the
carvings in three dimensions often represent the human figure,
whereas two-dimensional drawings, etchings, and paintings mostly
represent animals and are much more successful than the human
outlines. In the Magdalenian, miniature sculpture of animals was
added to that of the human figure (Fig. 21).
Success in seizing the salient outline was the earliest
characteristic of the paintings and drawings. The first Aurignacian
engravings are invariably in profile and usually show only the two
legs on the immediately visible side. In time the artists also learned
to suggest typical positions and movements—the motion of a
reindeer lowering its head to browse, the way an angry bull switches
his tail or paws the ground, the curl of the end of an elephant’s trunk
(Figs. 22, 24). In the Magdalenian, all four legs are usually depicted,
and the profile, although remaining most frequent, as it is most
characteristic, is no longer the only aspect. There are occasional
pictures of animals from before or behind, or of a reindeer with its
head turned backward.

Fig. 21. Horse carved in mammoth ivory. From Lourdes, France. The spirited
portrayal of the neck, ears, eyes, and mouth parts is characteristic of
Magdalenian sculpture.

There are also some devices which look like the beginnings of
attempts at composition. The effect of a row of reindeer is produced
by drawing out the first few in some detail, and then suggesting the
others by sketching in their horns (Fig. 23). Artists were no longer
content, in the Magdalenian, always to do each animal as a solitary,
static unit. They were trying, with some measure of success, to
represent the animals as they moved in life and perhaps to combine
several of them into one coherent picture or to suggest a setting.
By this time they had also acquired considerable ability in handling
colors. The Aurignacian and Solutrean artists restricted themselves
to monochrome effects. They engraved or painted outlines and
sometimes accentuated these by filling them in with pigment. But the
best of the later painters in the Magdalenian—those, for instance,
who left their frescoes on the walls of the famous cave of Altamira in
Spain—used three or four colors at once and blended these into
transition tones.

Fig. 22. Engraving of a charging mammoth. On a fragment of ivory tusk found at


La Madeleine, France. While the artist’s strokes were crude, he was able to
depict the animal’s action with remarkable vigor. Note the roll of the eye, the
flapping ears, the raised tail expressive of anger.

While animals constitute the subjects of probably four-fifths of the


specimens of Palæolithic art, and human beings most of the
remainder, representations of plants and unrealistic decorative
designs are known. The latter seem to have begun to be specially
prevalent in the latest Magdalenian, as if in preparation of the
conventionalized, non-naturalistic art of the transitional Azilian and
Neolithic.

81. Summary of Advance in the Palæolithic


The history of civilization has herewith been outlined from its first
dim beginnings to about twelve thousand years ago—say to the
neighborhood of 10,000 B.C., as the historian would put it. Progress
is immensely slow at the outset, but gradually speeds up. The
tabulation in Figure 25 summarizes some of the principal features of
this evolution. This diagram does not pretend to be complete; it does
try to include some of the most important and representative
inventions, arts, and accomplishments of the Old Stone Age.

Fig. 23. Magdalenian engraving of a herd of reindeer, found in the grotto of La


Mairie, France. The impressionistic manner enabled the artist to suggest
rather effectively a large herd while drawing out only four animals.

Thus it appears that the Chellean and Acheulean periods are


characterized essentially by a single art, that of chipping implements
on a core of flint, plus perhaps the use of fire. The Mousterian
evinces progress: stone tools are now made from the flake as well
as the core, possibly are sometimes hafted, bone is occasionally
utilized, and there are the first indications of budding religion; four or
five entries are required to represent these culture traits.
The greatest advance comes from the Mousterian to the
Aurignacian; in other words, between the Lower and the Upper
Palæolithic. Three times as many accomplishments are listed as in
the Mousterian, and whole series of new inventions are now first met
with: body ornaments, bone implements, æsthetic products. This
sudden leap in the figures goes far to signalize the importance of the
division between the Upper and the Lower Palæolithic. In the
Solutrean and Magdalenian still further inventions or refinements
appear, until, when the Old Stone Age comes to a close,[12] the stock
of human civilization may be described as perhaps twenty times as
rich as at the beginning. These figures are not to be taken too
literally. The tabulation could easily have been compiled on a more
elaborate basis. But even then the relative proportion of culture
features in each period would remain approximately as here given.
And as regards the general fact of accumulation of civilization, and
its range and nature, the diagram may be accepted as substantially
representative of what happened.

Fig. 24. Magdalenian engraving, perhaps a composition: browsing reindeer among


grass, reeds, and water. Note the naturalistic movement suggested by the
legs and position of the head. Engraved so as to encircle a piece of antler.
Found at Kesslerloch, Switzerland.
Fig. 25. Growth of civilization during the Palæolithic.

The end of the Palæolithic thus sees man in possession of a


number of mechanical arts which enable him to produce a
considerable variety of tools in several materials: sees him
controlling fire; cooking food; wearing clothes, and living in definite
habitations; probably possessing some sort of social grouping, order,
and ideas of law and justice; clearly under the influence of some kind
of religion; highly advanced in the plastic arts; and presumably
already narrating legends and singing songs. In short, many
fundamental elements of civilization were established. It is true that
the sum total of knowledge and accomplishments was still pitifully
small. The most advanced of the Old Stone Age men perhaps knew
and could do about one thing for every hundred that we know and
can do. A whole array of fundamental inventions—the bow and
arrow, pottery, domestication of animals and plants—had not yet
been attempted, and they do not appear on the scene until the
Neolithic. But in spite of the enormous gaps remaining to be filled in
the Neolithic and in the historic period, it does seem fair to say that
many of the outlines of what civilization was ultimately to be had
been substantially blocked out during the Upper Palæolithic. Most of
the framework was there, even though but a small fraction of its
content had yet been entered.
CHAPTER VII
HEREDITY, CLIMATE, AND CIVILIZATION

82. Heredity.—83. Geographical environment.—84. Diet.—85. Agriculture.


—86. Cultural factors.—87. Cultural distribution.—88. Historical
induction.

82. Heredity
The first of the several factors through which it is logically possible
to explain the life and conduct and customs of any people is race or
heredity: in other words, the inborn tendencies, bodily and mental, of
the people that carry these customs. At first sight it may seem that
this element of race might be quite influential. Since peoples differ in
inherited characteristics of body—complexion, features, hair, eye
color, head form, and the like—these bodily inherited peculiarities
ought to be accompanied by mentally inherited traits, such as
greater or less inclination to courage, energy, power of abstract
thought, mechanical ingenuity, musical or æsthetic proclivities, swift
reactions, ability to concentrate, gift of expression. Such racial
mental traits, again, might conceivably be expressed in the conduct
and culture of each people. Races born to a greater activity of the
mechanical faculties would achieve more or higher inventions, those
innately gifted in the direction of music would develop more subtly
melodious songs, and so on.
Yet in every particular case it is difficult or impossible to establish
by incontrovertible evidence that heredity is the specific cause of this
accomplishment, of this point of view, or of this mode of life; that it is
the determining factor to such and such degree of such and such
customs. This is not a denial of the probability that inborn racial
differences exist. It is an affirmation of the difficulty, discussed in
Chapters I, IV, and V, of knowing what is inborn; and more
specifically, of the difficulty of tracing particular customary activities
back to particular racial qualities. The problem of connecting specific
race traits with specific phenomena of culture or group conduct, such
as settled life, architecture in stone, religious symbolism, and the
like,—of determining how much of this type of architecture or
symbolism is instinctive in the race and how much of it is the result of
traditional or social influences,—remains unsolved.
For example, should one try to apply to the explanation of the
mode of life or culture of the Indians of the Southwestern United
States biological facts, such as their head form, one would be
confronted by the difficulty that long heads are characteristic of some
of the town-building tribes, or Pueblos, and also of some of the tribes
living in brush huts. Broad heads are also found among both the
settled and nomadic tribes. The Pueblo Taos and non-Pueblo Pima
are narrow-headed, the Pueblo Zuñi and non-Pueblo Apache broad-
headed. So with the pulse rate, which has been already mentioned
(§ 70) as unusually slow among the Southwestern Indians. It is the
same for the nomadic Apache who lived by fighting, and for the Hopi
and Zuñi who are famous for their timidity and gentleness. Similar
cases might be cited almost endlessly. It is evident that they are of a
kind with the lack of correspondence between race and speech, or
race and nationality, among the European peoples.

83. Geographical Environment


When it comes to the second factor by which culture might
theoretically be explained—physical environment or geography—
similar difficulties are encountered.
It is of course plain that a primitive tribe under the equator would
never invent the ice box, and that the Eskimo will not keep their food
and water in buckets of bamboo, although it is possible that if the
Eskimo had had bamboo carried to them by ocean currents, they
would have been both glad and able to use it. The materials and
opportunities provided by nature may be made use of by each
people, while other materials not being provided, other arts or
customs can therefore not be developed. But evidently this
correspondence is mainly negative. Not performing an act because
one lacks the opportunity by no means proves that the opportunity
will necessarily lead to the performance. Two nations will live where
there is ice to store and one will invent and the other fail to invent the
ice chest. Whole series of peoples possess bamboo and clay, and
yet some of them draw water in bamboo joints and others in pots.
Obviously, natural environment does impose certain limiting
conditions on human life; but equally obviously, it does not cause
inventions or institutions.
The native Australians have wood and cord and flint but do not
make bows and arrows. Their civilization had not advanced to the
point where they were able to devise an efficient bow, and the
requisite idea failed to be carried to them from elsewhere as it was to
other peoples who also did not invent the weapon. The Polynesians,
on the other hand, seem once to have had the weapon, as
evidenced by their retaining it as a toy, but to have disused it,
perhaps because they specialized on fighting with spears and clubs.
Modern civilized people fight at long range, but have let bows go out
of use, except for sport, because their knowledge of metallurgy and
chemistry centuries ago progressed to the point where they could
produce firearms. Development or lack of development or
specialization of other cultural activities—social causes—thus
determine more directly than other factors whether or not a people
employ the bow and arrow. Of those mentioned, the Australians are
the only ones with whom a factor of natural environment might be
alleged to enter: namely, their isolation, which cut them off from
communications and the opportunity to learn from other races. Yet
such isolation is as much a matter of inability to traverse space as it
is a matter of physical distance. A developed art of navigation would
have abolished the Australian isolation. Thus, this seemingly
environmental cause of a cultural fact depends for its effectiveness
on a co-existing cultural cause. It is the latter which is the most
immediate or specific cause.
In general, then, it may be concluded that the directly determining
factors of cultural phenomena are not nature which gives or
withholds materials, but the general state of knowledge and
technology and advancement of the group; in short, historical or
cultural influences.

84. Diet
The greater part of the Southwest is arid. Fish are scarce. The
result is that most of the tribes get little opportunity to fish. Most of
these Southwestern Indians will not eat fish; in fact, think them
poisonous. This circumstance might lead to the following inference:
nature does not furnish fish in abundance; therefore the Indians got
out of the habit of eating them, and finally came to believe them
poisonous. At first blush this may seem a sufficient explanation. But
it is well to note that the explanation has two parts and that only one
of them has to do with nature: the habit of not eating fish because
they are too scarce to make it worth while. As soon as one proceeds
to the second step, that the disuse led to aversion and then to a false
belief of poisonousness, one has gone on to a different matter.
Disuse, aversion, and belief lie wholly within the field of human
conduct. To derive a psychological phenomenon, such as a belief,
from another psychological phenomenon such as a particular disuse,
because this disuse is founded on a geographical factor, would of
course be a logical fallacy. It can also be shown not to hold, since we
prize caviar and oysters and venison in proportion to their rarity.
Scarcity in this case thus leads to the contrary psychological attitude,
and either fails to establish beliefs or establishes favorable ones.
Again, either through a change in climate or through the
improvement of trade, a food that was scarce may become plentiful.
Or a people may remove to a new habitat, different from that in
which their customs of eating were formed. If environment alone
were the dominating cause of their customs, these customs should
then immediately alter. As a fact, a group sometimes adheres to its
old customs. The immediate cause of such conservatism is habit or
inertia or inclination toward superstition or fear of taboo, all of which
are mental reactions expressed in folkways or social customs. Thus
environment remains at most a partial and indirectly operating
cause.
A case in point is that of the Jews. It is often said that the Jew’s
prohibition against eating pork and oysters and lobsters originated in
hygienic considerations; that these were climatically unsafe foods for
him in Palestine. This explanation is more simple than true. Ancient
Palestine was an arid country in which hogs could not be raised with
economic profit, and so they were not raised; and the Philistine and
Phœnician kept the Jew from the coast along which he might have
obtained shellfish. Eating neither food, he happened to acquire a
distrust of them; having the distrust, he rationalized it by saying that
it was foreign and wicked and irreligious to act counter to his habits
—just like the Pueblo Indian; and in the end had the Lord issue the
prohibition for him. Yet this outcome is a long way from the starting
point of natural environment. The environment may indeed be said to
have furnished the first occasion, but the determining causes of the
taboos in the Mosaic law are of an entirely different kind—distrust,
custom, rationalization, psychological or cultural factors. If doubt
remains, it is dispelled by the orthodox Jew of to-day, whose
environment thrusts some of his forbidden foods at him as
economically and hygienically satisfactory, whereas he still shudders
at the thought of tasting them.
If this sort of cultural crystallizing of custom and subsequent
rationalizing or ritual sanctioning takes place among civilized and
intelligent people, the like must occur among uncivilized tribes.

85. Agriculture
Attempts have been made to derive the invention of agriculture
from climatic factors. The first theory was that farming took its rise in
the tropics, where agriculture came naturally, almost without effort,
under a bounteous sky. Only after people had acquired the habit of
farming and had moved into other less favorably endowed countries,
did they take their agriculture seriously in order to survive. But a
second, equally plausible, and quite contradictory theory has been
advanced, which looks toward the duress rather than the easy favors
of nature. On the basis of conditions among the modern Papago
Indians and the ancient inhabitants of the Southwest, it has been
argued that it must have been the peoples of arid countries who
invented agriculture, necessity driving them to it through shortage of
wild supplies.
Between such flat opposites, the choice is merely one of
unscientific guessing. In this particular case of the Southwest it is
certain that both guesses are wrong. Agriculture did not come to the
natives of this area because nature was favorable or because it was
unfavorable. It came because through increase of knowledge and
change of attitude, some people in the region of Southern Mexico or
Guatemala or beyond first turned agriculturists, and from them the
art was gradually carried, through nation after nation, to the
Southwestern tribes, and finally even to the Indians of the North
Atlantic coast.
The reasons for acceptance of this explanation are several. First is
the distribution of native agriculture, whose practice was about
equally spread in the two American continents with its middle in or
near Central America. If a geographical diffusion of the art from a
center took place, its radiation or extension would probably be about
equal to the north and south. Then, the middle portions of the new
world held the greatest concentration of native population, such as
would have tended to produce a pressure in the direction of the
establishment of agriculture and would also normally be a
consequence of the continued custom of farming, as opposed to
unsettled life. Again, the Southwestern tribes planted only maize,
beans, and squashes; the Mexicans grew in addition tomatoes, chili
peppers, cacao, and sweet potatoes. It looks as if they had carried
their agriculture farther through having been at it longer. Then,
pottery has evidently spread out from the same center, and the two
arts seem to go hand in hand. Other evidence might be adduced,
such as archæological excavations and the botanical fact that the
home of the nearest wild relatives of the plants cultivated in the
Southwest is the central or middle American area (§ 183).
In short, the Southwestern Indians did not farm because nature
induced them to make the invention. They did not make the invention
at all. A far away people made it, and from them it was transmitted to
the Southwest through a series of successive tribal contacts. These
contacts, which then are the specific cause of Southwestern
agriculture, constitute a human social factor; a cultural or
civilizational factor. Climatic or physical environment did not enter
into the matter at all, except to render agriculture somewhat difficult
in the arid Southwest, though not difficult enough to prevent it. Had
the Southwest been thoroughly desert, agriculture could not have got
a foot-hold there. But this would be only a limiting condition; the
active or positive causes that brought about the Southwestern
agriculture are its invention farther South, the spread of the invention
to the North, and its acceptance there.
Of course this conclusion sheds no light on the causes of the first
invention in the middle American region. The ultimate origin of the
phenomenon has not been penetrated. But the prevalence of
agriculture in the aboriginal Southwest for several thousand years
past has been pretty certainly accounted for, and by an explanation
in terms of culture or civilization, or the activity of societies of human
beings.

86. Cultural Factors


Such cultural causes constitute the third set or kind of factors by
which civilization is explainable. If the example just discussed is
representative, it is clear that cultural factors ordinarily interpret more
phenomena of civilization, and interpret them more fully, than factors
either of racial heredity or physical environment.
It is different in zoölogy and botany. The forms and behavior of
animals and plants are explainable in terms of heredity and
environment because animals and plants have no culture. It is true
that the forms and behavior are determined also by other animals
and plants, their characteristics, habits, and abundance, but these
factors are in a larger sense part of the environment. They are at any
rate sub-cultural. But since anthropology deals with beings whose
distinctive trait in social relations is the possession of the thing that
we call culture, the factors which biology employs are insufficient. It
is not that heredity and natural environment fail to apply to man, but
that they apply only indirectly and remotely to his civilization. This
fundamental fact has often been overlooked, especially in modern
times, because the biological sciences having achieved successful
increases of knowledge and understanding, the temptation was great
to borrow their method outright and apply it without serious
modification to the human material of anthropology. This procedure
simplified the situation, but yielded inadequate and illusory results.
For a very long time the idea that man possessed and animals
lacked a soul influenced people’s thought to such a degree that they
scarcely thought of human beings in terms of biological causality, of
heredity and environment. Then when a reaction began to set in,
less than two centuries ago, and it became more generally
recognized that man was an animal, the pendulum swung to the
other extreme and the tendency grew of seeing in him only the
animal, the cultureless being, and of either ignoring his culture or
thinking that it could be explained away by resolving it into the
factors familiar from biology. The just and wise course lies between.
The biological aspects of man must be interpreted in terms of
biological causation, his cultural aspects in terms first of all of cultural
causation. After they have been thus resolved, the cultural causes
may reduce to ultimate factors of heredity and natural environment.

87. Cultural Distribution


The Southwest also provides an example of how cultural
phenomena can be seen to be arranged geographically so as to
yield a meaning or to outline their history, without reference to
climate or natural influences. Near the center of the area, in northern
New Mexico and Arizona, live four groups of Pueblo or town building
Indians—the Hopi, Zuñi, Keres, and Tewa or Tano—who represent a
sort of élite of the native culture. They farm, make pottery,
accumulate wealth in turquoise, are governed by priests, worship
under a remarkably complex set of rituals, which involve altars,
masks, symbols of all sorts, and a rude sort of philosophy.
As one goes from the Pueblo center to the less settled tribes, one
encounters first the Navaho, who are earth hut builders and farm but
little, yet share much of the Pueblo elaborateness of ritual, including
altars, masks, and symbols. A little farther out, among the Apache
and Pima, the cults have perceptibly diminished in intricacy and
symbolic value: altars and masks are lacking.
The simplification increases among the more remote Mohave,
whose cults are based on dreams instead of priestly tradition. Still
farther, on the shores of the Pacific among the Luiseño and
Gabrielino, some Pueblo traits can still be found; cult altars and
pottery, for instance. But agriculture, homes of stone, turquoise,
priests, and the majority of Pueblo institutions are unknown. Finally,
still farther away in central California, the Yokuts now and then show
a culture trait reminiscent of the Pueblos: grooved arrow
straighteners, perhaps, or occasional rudely made pottery vessels.
These are suggestive bits; fragments that have been whittled away
or toned down. Pueblo culture as a whole has vanished at this
distance. In its place the Yokuts possess quite different arts and
institutions and beliefs.
What is the significance of this gradual fading away of one type of
civilization and its replacement by others? Evidently that certain
influences have radiated out from the higher Pueblo center, and that
the effect of these has diminished in proportion to the number of
tribes they have passed through. The Pueblos have succeeded in
handing over the largest share of their civilization to the adjacent
Navaho—and no doubt also received most from them. The Apache
being more remote, were less affected; and so on to the farthest
limits of the influences.
It is also clear that a time element is involved. A people receiving
an art from another obviously acquires this later than the inventors.
Most traits which the central Pueblos share with peripheral tribes
may be assumed to have existed longer among the Pueblos, simply
because they possess more traits in their culture and the flow has
prevailingly been out from them. Thus they make uncolored, two-
colored, and three-colored pottery; the tribes on the margin of the
Southwest, uncolored pottery only; those beyond the range of
immediate Southwestern influence, no pottery at all. Unless
therefore there should be special reasons suggestive of a
degenerative loss of the art among the marginal tribes—and no such
reasons are known—the conclusion is forced that Southwestern
pottery was first made by the ancestors of the Pueblos or their
predecessors in the central part of the area, presumably as plain
ware, and that thence knowledge of the art was gradually carried
outward. However while simple pottery making was thus being taken
up by the tribes nearest to the Pueblo district, the Pueblos were
going ahead and learning to ornament vessels with painted designs.
In time this added art also spread to the neighbors, but meanwhile
these had passed knowledge of the first stage on to the tribes still
farther out than themselves; and meanwhile also the Pueblos had
perhaps gone on to a third stage, that of combining colors in their
decoration.
In this way, if nothing interrupted the even regularity of the
process, the focal people, with their lead in creating or inventing or
improving, might pass through half a dozen successive stages of the
art, or of many arts, while the outermost peoples were just beginning
to receive the rudiments. The intermediate tribes would show
attainment of a less or greater number of stages in proportion to their
distance from the center. In this event the main facts concerning the
pottery art of the Southwest could be represented by a diagram of a
step pyramid, each level or step picturing a new increment to the
basic art. The Pueblos would be at the peak of the pyramid, five or
six steps high, the near-by tribes a step or two lower; and so on to
the outermost, who remain at, or have only recently attained to, the
first or lowest level; while beyond these would be the non-pottery-
making tribes wholly outside the Pueblo sphere of influence.
Of course on the actual map the distribution of the various forms
or stages of pottery made does not work out with the perfect
regularity of our schematic diagram. Here and there a tribe has
migrated from its habitat and disturbed the symmetry of
arrangement; or the population of a district has been so thin that it
could live on wild products without resorting to agriculture, so that it
remained more or less nomadic and had no use for fragile pottery; or
a third group of tribes developed basket making to a pitch which
yielded excellent vessels, with the result that they were satisfied and
failed to take up pottery, or took it up half-heartedly, so that the art
Welcome to our website – the ideal destination for book lovers and
knowledge seekers. With a mission to inspire endlessly, we offer a
vast collection of books, ranging from classic literary works to
specialized publications, self-development books, and children's
literature. Each book is a new journey of discovery, expanding
knowledge and enriching the soul of the reade

Our website is not just a platform for buying books, but a bridge
connecting readers to the timeless values of culture and wisdom. With
an elegant, user-friendly interface and an intelligent search system,
we are committed to providing a quick and convenient shopping
experience. Additionally, our special promotions and home delivery
services ensure that you save time and fully enjoy the joy of reading.

Let us accompany you on the journey of exploring knowledge and


personal growth!

ebookmass.com

You might also like